Digital Insights
The spread of misinformation: A multivariate analysis of the relationship between individual characteristics and fact-checking behaviours of Canadians
by Howard Bilodeau and Aisha Khalid
Skip to text
Text begins
Abstract
- In 2023, over two in five Canadians (43%) thought it was becoming harder to distinguish between true and false news or information, compared with three years prior. This follows the finding that, in 2022, almost three-quarters of Canadians (73%) reported having seen content online that they suspected to be false or inaccurate in the past 12 months.
- In this paper, multivariate analyses were conducted to examine the role of demographic and socioeconomic characteristics in the likelihood of fact-checking news or information and sharing unverified online information, behaviours thought to be associated with the spread of misinformation.
- Age and education level emerge as factors that are likely to be important in the spread of misinformation. Increasing age was associated with a lower probability of fact-checking information. It was also associated with a lower probability of sharing unverified online information. Conversely, higher education was associated with a higher likelihood of engaging in both behaviours.
- The results of this paper demonstrate the complexity surrounding the decisions to fact-check information. Because the surveys used in this analysis did not directly assess the ability of respondents to identify misinformation, further research is required to determine how fact-checking behaviours relate to the spread of misinformation.
Introduction
As technology evolves, there are more ways than ever to access news and information. While Canadians continue to use more traditional information sources, such as news websites, television, and radio, social media platforms (e.g., Facebook, Instagram, TikTok, YouTube) are becoming increasingly common sources of information. A recently released wave of Statistics Canada’s Survey Series on People and their Communities (SSPC) found that over two in five Canadians (44%) typicallyNote 1 got their news or information in 2023 from social media accounts unaffiliated with government, scientific or news organizations. The percentage of Canadians typically using social media as an information source was larger than the proportion of those typically turning to federal government sources (33%), provincial or municipal government sources (33%), and scientific experts or peer-reviewed journals (26%). Meanwhile, Canadians were more likely to typically get information from news organizations (76%) and close contacts (53%).
Although the Internet has made it easier than ever to get information, it has also created new opportunities for misinformation to spread. Over two in five Canadians (43%) reported on the SSPC that it was becoming harder to distinguish between true and false news or information in 2023, compared with three years prior. This follows the finding from Statistics Canada’s 2022 Canadian Internet Use Survey (CIUS) that almost three-quarters of Canadians (73%) had seen content online that they suspected to be false or inaccurate in the past 12 months.
Foreign states and ideologically motivated individuals and groups are increasingly leveraging new and evolving technologies like artificial intelligence and social media algorithms to spread misinformation.Note 2 Misinformation can make it difficult for individuals to make informed decisions in areas such as health, finances and politics, and can pose a threat to democracy.Note 3 On the SSPC, 59% of Canadians reported that they were very or extremely concerned about online misinformationNote 4 in 2023.
This paper elaborates on insights from the SSPC and the CIUS by conducting multivariate analyses to further examine the role of demographic and socioeconomic characteristics on self-reported behaviours thought to be associated with the spread of misinformation. The first section of this paper provides a general overview of the levels of trust Canadians have in various information sources. The second section uses data from the SSPC to examine how individual characteristics affect the likelihood of individuals verifying the accuracy of information. The third uses data from the CIUS to look at how individual characteristics affect the likelihood of sharing unverified online information. Finally, the “Analysis” section examines the results of all the multivariate analyses by focusing on the explanatory variables common to both surveys.
Trust in news or information sources
While many Canadians now use social media as a source of news or information, it is still among their least trusted information sources. The SSPC found that, in 2023, almost two-thirds of Canadians (64%) reported low levels of trust in social media posts by accounts unaffiliated with government, scientific or news organizations as sources of information.Note 5 Conversely, the sources Canadians most commonly reported trusting were scientific experts or peer-reviewed journals (68%), close contacts (56%) and the federal government (50%). Chart 1 presents the reported levels of trust for all news or information sources covered in the SSPC.
Data table for Chart 1
News or information source | Can be trusted | Neutral | Cannot be trusted | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
percent | 95% confidence interval | percent | 95% confidence interval | percent | 95% confidence interval | ||||
from | to | from | to | from | to | ||||
Notes: The trust question on the Survey Series on People and their Communities used a scale from “cannot be trusted at all” (1) to “can be trusted completely” (5). For this chart, responses were reclassified as follows: 1 to 2 = cannot be trusted, 3 = neutral, and 4 to 5 = can be trusted. Error bars represent the 95% confidence intervals.
Source: Statistics Canada, Survey Series on People and their Communities, wave 3, 2023. |
|||||||||
Scientific experts or peer-reviewed journals | 68 | 64 | 72 | 22 | 20 | 24 | 11 | 9 | 13 |
Close contacts | 56 | 53 | 60 | 36 | 34 | 38 | 8 | 7 | 10 |
Federal government communications | 50 | 46 | 53 | 28 | 27 | 30 | 22 | 19 | 25 |
Provincial, territorial or municipal government communications | 47 | 44 | 50 | 32 | 30 | 34 | 21 | 19 | 24 |
News organizations | 37 | 34 | 40 | 39 | 37 | 41 | 25 | 22 | 28 |
Other Internet sources | 19 | 17 | 21 | 46 | 44 | 48 | 35 | 32 | 39 |
Social media posts by other users | 7 | 6 | 8 | 29 | 27 | 31 | 64 | 60 | 68 |
Having low levels of trust towards certain news or information sources may be related to the prevalence of misinformation online. The 2022 CIUS found that just over one-quarter of Canadians (27%) saw content online that they suspected to be false or inaccurate every day. However, it is important to note that individuals are not always able to accurately identify misinformation, even when they fact-check. When fact-checking information, individuals can fall victim to confirmation bias, which is the tendency of a person to seek and assign greater importance to information that supports their existing beliefs.
As an example of the difficulty in accurately spotting misinformation, the Survey of Online Harms in Canada, conducted in 2022 by the Dais at Toronto Metropolitan University, found that almost half of respondents (47%) were unable to correctly identify at least 75% of a series of misinformation statements they were presented with. Meanwhile, about one in six respondents (15%) could correctly identify 25% or fewer of the misinformation statements. This latter group was “less trusting of mainstream news, more likely to trust social media and use it for news, and less likely to fact-check.”Note 6 Similarly, in the global Truth Quest Survey conducted by the Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development (OECD) in 2024, respondents, on average, misidentified true and false content they were presented with 40% of the time. Additionally, more trust in social media as a news source was associated with a reduced ability to correctly identify content.Note 7
Survey Series on People and their Communities regressions: Likelihood of fact-checking news or information
While the SSPC did not directly measure belief in misinformation, it did examine the likelihood of verifying the accuracy of news stories about current issues using at least one additional source. In 2023, over half of Canadians (53%) said they always or often fact-check news or information, while a minority (4%) reported never fact-checking it (see Chart 2).
Data table for Chart 2
Frequency | Percent | 95% confidence interval | |
---|---|---|---|
from | to | ||
Note: Error bars represent the 95% confidence intervals.
Source: Statistics Canada, Survey Series on People and their Communities, wave 3, 2023. |
|||
Never | 4 | 3 | 5 |
Rarely | 11 | 10 | 12 |
Sometimes | 32 | 30 | 34 |
Often | 36 | 34 | 38 |
Always | 17 | 16 | 19 |
Among the more than four in five Canadians (83%) who do not always fact-check information, half (50%) reported a lack of interest or motivation as a reason for not doing so. To further investigate the factors associated with more verification of information, weighted ordinal logistic regression analyses were performed using data from the SSPC. Table 1 in the “Regression results” section at the end of the paper presents the explanatory variables analyzed and their definitions. Significant results are highlighted with asterisks, and results are presented as odds ratios.Note 8
In the first specification, only demographic and socioeconomic characteristics were included as explanatory variables. The coefficients for age and education were found to be statistically significant. The likelihood of fact-checking news or information was found to decrease as age increased, while those who had higher levels of education had a greater likelihood of doing so. Among racialized groups, the odds ratios for the Chinese, Filipino and Korean groups were significant and associated with a lower likelihood of fact-checking news or information.
To examine other factors associated with the likelihood of verifying news stories, a second specification, which also included mental health measures and variables related to news or information consumption, was conducted. In this specification, the coefficients for racialized groups were no longer significant. The coefficient for education remained significant, but its estimated effect was reduced, while the relationship between age and the likelihood of fact-checking news or information remained significant and of the same magnitude.
Misinformation is often designed to trigger an emotional response, motivating a person to quickly share the information without fact-checking it.Note 3 While the SSPC did not measure the current emotional state of respondents, it did include self-reported mental health measures. These measures were included in the second specification as proxies for the effect of the emotional response on fact-checking behaviours. Among the mental health variables examined, hopefulness and life satisfaction were statistically significant. A more hopeful view of the future was associated with a greater likelihood of fact-checking news or information, while greater life satisfaction was associated with a lower probability of doing so.Note 9
In terms of the news or information consumption variables, having a greater concern about misinformation was statistically significantly associated with a higher probability of fact-checking. The coefficients for typically using federal, expert or other Internet information sources were also significant. For all these information sources, typical use was linked to a greater likelihood of fact-checking. Similarly, the coefficients for having trust in expert and other Internet sources were significant and associated with a higher probability of fact-checking. These findings may be the result of fact-checkers typically using expert and other Internet sources to verify information they encounter elsewhere. However, because the SSPC did not specifically ask individuals how they conducted their fact-checking, it is impossible to definitively determine the cause of these results.
The coefficient for typical use of news organizations as an information source was also significant and positively associated with fact-checking behaviour. However, greater trust in news organizations was statistically significantly associated with a lower probability of fact-checking. These results are likely related to the relatively even distribution of Canadians who trust, distrust or have neutral feelings of trust towards news organizations (as was shown in Chart 1). In addition to news organizations, the estimate for trust in close contacts was also significant and associated with a lower probability of fact-checking news or information, demonstrating how social relationships can play an important role in how information spreads.
Canadian Internet Use Survey regressions: Likelihood of sharing online news or information without fact-checking it
Misinformation is of most concern when it spreads unchecked. The 2022 CIUS found that almost one in seven Canadians (14%) had shared at least some online news or information without checking its accuracy in the past 12 months (see Chart 3).
Data table for Chart 3
Frequency | Percent | 95% confidence interval | |
---|---|---|---|
from | to | ||
Notes: The chart categories do not sum to 100% because of rounding and the exclusion of Internet non-users (5% of Canadians). Error bars represent the 95% confidence intervals.
Source: Statistics Canada, Canadian Internet Use Survey, 2022. |
|||
Not in the past 12 months | 81 | 80 | 81 |
Rarely | 3 | 3 | 4 |
Sometimes | 6 | 6 | 6 |
Often | 3 | 3 | 3 |
Always | 1 | 1 | 1 |
To further examine the factors that predict the likelihood of sharing online information without fact-checking it, weighted ordinal logistic regressions were conducted using different combinations of explanatory variables from the CIUS (see Table 2 in the “Regression results” section). As in the previous section, odds ratios were used to interpret the results.
In the first specification, only demographic and socioeconomic characteristics were examined. The coefficient for age was found to be statistically significant, with increasing age resulting in a lower probability of sharing online information without fact-checking it. The estimates for family income and education were also significant, with higher values for both resulting in a higher probability of sharing information without fact-checking it. Among racialized groups, the coefficients for the Chinese and South Asian groups were significant. The Chinese group was associated with a higher likelihood of sharing unverified information, while the South Asian group was associated with a lower likelihood.
To examine other factors suspected of being related to fact-checking and sharing information, an additional specification was conducted that incorporated mental health variables, a variable for time spent online for general purposesNote 10 and a variable for having previously seen misinformation. When these variables were included in the regression, education was no longer a significant predictor of sharing information without fact-checking it. However, all other variables that were previously significant remained significant.
As with the SSPC regressions, self-reported mental health measures were included in the second specification as proxies for the effect of the emotional response on fact-checking and information-sharing behaviours. In this specification, it was found that better mental health statistically significantly decreased the likelihood of sharing unverified online information. However, unlike in the previous section, life satisfaction was not significant. Time spent online was found to also be significant, with spending more time online increasing the likelihood of sharing information without fact-checking it. The odds ratio for seen misinformation was also greater than one and significant.
Because the ability to identify misinformation has been previously shown to be related to education level in some cases,Note 11 a third specification was conducted. This specification included all the variables from the second specification, except for seen misinformation, to see how the correlation between that variable and education affected the results. In the third specification, the coefficient for education was once again significant, with an identical odds ratio to the first specification. This confirms that the lack of significance for the education variable in the second specification was connected to its relationship with seen misinformation.
Analysis
When the results of both surveys are examined, age and education level emerge as factors that are likely to be important in the spread of misinformation. The probability of fact-checking news or information was found to decrease with increasing age, but so too did the likelihood of sharing unverified online information. These findings may be related to the fact that older Canadians have shown a greater preference for non-Internet information sources, compared with other age groups.Note 12 It can be more challenging to fact-check and share information when less time is spent online. This idea is further supported by the finding in this paper that spending more time online was associated with a higher likelihood of sharing information without fact-checking it.
Those who were more educated had a higher probability of fact-checking news or information but were also more likely to share online information without fact-checking it. This latter finding may be the result of those who are more educated generally feeling more confident in their ability to correctly identify false or misleading information, as was shown in the OECD’s Truth Quest Survey.Note 7 This is also supported by the fact that, in the CIUS regressions, the odds ratio for education was not statistically significant when controlling for having previously seen misinformation. However, it is important to note that neither the SSPC nor the CIUS measured the accuracy of people’s ability to spot misinformation. Therefore, these surveys cannot determine whether confidence in identifying misinformation was warranted. The Truth Quest Survey explored this concept and found no relationship between confidence in spotting misinformation and the ability to do so.Note 7
Mixed results were found regarding the relationship between mental health measures, fact-checking online information and sharing it without fact-checking it. Higher life satisfaction was associated with a lower probability of fact-checking, while no significant relationship between life satisfaction and sharing unverified information could be found. Conversely, greater hopefulness was associated with a higher probability of fact-checking, and better mental health was associated with a lower likelihood of sharing information without fact-checking it. These results may indicate that the emotional response to misinformation has a differing effect on the decisions to fact-check information and share it without fact-checking it. However, it is impossible to definitively determine the cause of these findings because the mental health measures examined in the SSPC and CIUS were different. Additionally, the mixed results may also have been because some of the examined mental health measures were inaccurate proxies for the emotional state of a person at the time they made the decision whether to fact-check news or information.
The SSPC and CIUS analyses also found some evidence that belonging to the Chinese group predicted a lower likelihood of fact-checking online information and a higher likelihood of sharing it without fact-checking it. However, the results for the likelihood of fact-checking information were not significant when controlling for mental health measures and variables related to news or information consumption. Researchers have previously noted that Chinese Americans often use social media apps that are distinct from those used by other groups.Note 13 Unique features and the cultural norms of users can lead to information spreading differently on different social media apps. This effect may also have contributed to the results observed in the SSPC and the CIUS.
Conclusion and limitations
Overall, these results demonstrate the complexity surrounding the decisions to fact-check information and share unverified findings. One limitation of the SSPC and the CIUS is that they focused only on self-reported fact-checking behaviours. This could have led some respondents to report how they wanted to be perceived, rather than how they behaved. This effect may have been particularly relevant for the CIUS regressions, where the dependent variable was skewed towards not having shared online information without fact-checking it in the past 12 months (as was shown in Chart 3). The skewed distribution may have also been related to the tiered structure of the survey questions used to define the dependent variable (as described in the notes of Table 2).
Regarding the CIUS, it is also important to note that only online news or information were in scope. The spread of misinformation online is of growing importance, but the Internet is not the only medium through which misinformation can travel. Future surveys could take a broader lens and collect measures related to sharing information obtained offline.
Another important limitation is that both surveys looked only at fact-checking behaviours and did not directly measure the spread of misinformation. While sharing information without fact-checking it is assumed to increase the possibility of spreading misinformation, the degree to which this occurs cannot be definitively determined with these surveys. Moreover, while the SSPC did allow for the examination of the relationship between the use of particular information sources and the decision to fact-check, it did not ask Canadians which sources they used for fact-checking. Because of confirmation bias, the sources used for fact-checking are another important consideration in the spread of misinformation. This topic has already begun to be explored by other researchers, such as the Information Overload report produced by the Canadian Association of Science Centres in 2022 and 2023. According to this report, many respondents found the search for truth to be a complex and iterative process wherein knowledge is refined over time by considering many different factors and information sources.Note 14 Future surveys could further examine how Canadians fact-check information and investigate which fact-checking strategies improve their ability to identify misinformation.
Start of text boxOther articles based on wave 3 of the Survey Series on People and their Communities and the 2022 Canadian Internet Use Survey
Results for wave 3 of the SSPC were released on December 20, 2023, in an article titled “Concerns with misinformation online, 2023.”
Results for the 2022 CIUS were released on July 20, 2023, in an article titled “Canadian Internet Use Survey, 2022.”
Digital economy and society statistics portal
Visit the Digital economy and society statistics portal to find data, publications and interactive tools related to the digital economy and society in one convenient location.
Regression results
Explanatory variable | Variable definition | Specification 1: Demographic and socioeconomic variables only | Specification 2: Demographic, socioeconomic, mental health, and news or information consumption variables |
---|---|---|---|
Odds ratio | |||
The reference categories for particular dummy variables are the inverse populations for those variables and are not shown in the table (e.g., non-racialized, non-Indigenous). Source: Statistics Canada, Survey Series on People and their Communities, wave 3, 2023. |
|||
Demographic and socioeconomic variables | |||
Age | Age (value) | 0.99 Table 1 Note ** | 0.99 Table 1 Note ** |
Male Table 1 Note 1 | Identifies as male (dummy variable) | 1.16 | 1.09 |
Education Table 1 Note 2 | Highest level of education (scale of 1 to 7) | 1.17 Table 1 Note ** | 1.07 Table 1 Note ** |
Born outside Canada | Born outside Canada (dummy variable) | 1.01 | 1.05 |
Personal income | After-tax personal income brackets (scale of 1 to 12) | 1.00 | 0.99 |
Location | Individuals residing within a census metropolitan area or census agglomeration (dummy variable) | 1.14 | 1.19 |
Racialized Table 1 Note 3 and IndigenousTable 1 Note 4 groups (dummy variables) | |||
Black | Classified as Black | 0.89 | 0.95 |
Chinese | Classified as Chinese | 0.72 Table 1 Note ** | 0.88 |
Filipino | Classified as Filipino | 0.81 Table 1 Note * | 1.01 |
Korean | Classified as Korean | 0.70 Table 1 Note * | 1.00 |
Japanese | Classified as Japanese | 0.87 | 1.05 |
Latin American | Classified as Latin American | 1.07 | 1.16 |
Southeast Asian | Classified as Southeast Asian | 0.87 | 1.05 |
South Asian | Classified as South Asian | 0.92 | 0.98 |
West Asian | Classified as West Asian | 0.95 | 1.11 |
Other racialized groups | Classified as other or multiple racialized groups | 0.89 | 0.83 |
Indigenous | Identifies as First Nations, Métis or Inuk (Inuit) | 0.81 | 0.71 |
Mental health variables | |||
Mental health conditions | Having emotional, psychological or mental health conditions (dummy variable) | ... not applicable | 1.06 |
Loneliness | Frequency of feeling lonely (scale of 1 to 5) | ... not applicable | 0.93 |
Hopefulness | Frequency of having a hopeful view of the future (scale of 1 to 5) | ... not applicable | 1.17 Table 1 Note ** |
Worthwhileness | Extent of feeling the things done in life are worthwhile (scale of 0 to 10) | ... not applicable | 1.00 |
Life satisfaction | Level of life satisfaction (scale of 0 to 10) | ... not applicable | 0.91 Table 1 Note ** |
Family satisfaction | Level of satisfaction with personal relationships—Family (scale of 0 to 10) | ... not applicable | 0.99 |
Friend satisfaction | Level of satisfaction with personal relationships—Friends (scale of 0 to 10) | ... not applicable | 0.99 |
News or information consumption variables | |||
Misinformation concern | Level of concern about the presence of misinformation online (scale of 1 to 5) | ... not applicable | 1.41 Table 1 Note ** |
Difficulty distinguishing truth | Level of difficulty distinguishing between true and false news or information, compared with three years ago (scale of 1 to 3) | ... not applicable | 1.03 |
News or information sources (dummy variables) | |||
Close contacts | Typically gets news or information from close contacts | ... not applicable | 0.89 |
Federal | Typically gets news or information from federal government communications | ... not applicable | 1.39 Table 1 Note * |
Provincial, territorial or municipal | Typically gets news or information from provincial, territorial or municipal government communications | ... not applicable | 1.14 |
Expert | Typically gets news or information from scientific experts or peer-reviewed journals | ... not applicable | 2.13 Table 1 Note ** |
News organization | Typically gets news or information from news organizations | ... not applicable | 1.28 Table 1 Note * |
Social media | Typically gets news or information from social media posts by other users | ... not applicable | 0.97 |
Other Internet | Typically gets news or information from other Internet sources | ... not applicable | 1.34 Table 1 Note ** |
Trust in news or information sources (scale of 1 to 5) | |||
Close contacts | Trust in close contacts as a source of news or information | ... not applicable | 0.89 Table 1 Note * |
Federal | Trust in federal government communications as a source of news or information | ... not applicable | 0.99 |
Provincial | Trust in provincial, territorial or municipal government communications as a source of news or information | ... not applicable | 0.94 |
Expert | Trust in scientific experts or peer-reviewed journals as a source of news or information | ... not applicable | 1.25 Table 1 Note ** |
News organization | Trust in news organizations as a source of news or information | ... not applicable | 0.75 Table 1 Note ** |
Social media | Trust in social media posts by other users as a source of news or information | ... not applicable | 1.01 |
Other Internet | Trust in other Internet sources as a source of news or information | ... not applicable | 1.13 Table 1 Note * |
Explanatory variable | Variable definition | Specification 1: Demographic and socioeconomic variables only | Specification 2: Demographic, socioeconomic and other variables | Specification 3: Demographic, socioeconomic and other variables, excluding seen misinformation |
---|---|---|---|---|
Odds ratio | ||||
The reference categories for particular dummy variables are the inverse populations for those variables and are not shown in the table (e.g., non-racialized, non-Indigenous). Source: Statistics Canada, Canadian Internet Use Survey, 2022. |
||||
Demographic and socioeconomic variables | ||||
Age | Age (value) | 0.98 Table 2 Note ** | 0.99 Table 2 Note ** | 0.99 Table 2 Note ** |
Male Table 2 Note 1 | Identifies as male (dummy variable) | 0.92 | 0.89 | 0.92 |
Education Table 2 Note 2 | Highest level of education (scale of 1 to 7) | 1.05 Table 2 Note ** | 1.03 | 1.05 Table 2 Note ** |
Immigrant Table 2 Note 3 | Landed immigrants admitted to Canada since 1952 (dummy variable) | 0.92 | 0.96 | 0.94 |
Family income | Census family income decile (scale of 1 to 10) | 1.02 Table 2 Note * | 1.03 Table 2 Note * | 1.03 Table 2 Note ** |
Location | Individuals residing within a census metropolitan area or census agglomeration (dummy variable) | 1.15 | 1.10 | 1.10 |
Racialized Table 2 Note 4 and IndigenousTable 2 Note 5 groups (dummy variables) | ||||
Black | Classified as Black | 0.98 | 1.02 | 0.95 |
Chinese | Classified as Chinese | 1.43 Table 2 Note ** | 1.47 Table 2 Note ** | 1.35 Table 2 Note * |
Filipino | Classified as Filipino | 0.86 | 0.92 | 0.86 |
Korean | Classified as Korean | 0.89 | 1.01 | 0.86 |
Japanese | Classified as Japanese | 0.88 | 1.00 | 0.94 |
Latin American | Classified as Latin American | 0.89 | 0.96 | 0.91 |
Southeast Asian | Classified as Southeast Asian | 1.20 | 1.26 | 1.13 |
South Asian | Classified as South Asian | 0.64 Table 2 Note ** | 0.71 Table 2 Note * | 0.64 Table 2 Note ** |
West Asian | Classified as West Asian | 1.15 | 1.26 | 1.17 |
Other racialized groups | Classified as other or multiple racialized groups | 0.91 | 0.93 | 0.88 |
Indigenous | Identifies as First Nations, Métis or Inuk (Inuit) | 1.32 | 1.29 | 1.27 |
Other variables | ||||
Time spent online | Time spent online per week on general Internet use Table 2 Note 6 (scale of 1 to 4) | ... not applicable | 1.18 Table 2 Note ** | 1.24 Table 2 Note ** |
Seen misinformation | Frequency of seeing information suspected to be false or inaccurate in the past 12 months (scale of 1 to 5) | ... not applicable | 1.28 Table 2 Note ** | ... not applicable |
Mental health | Perceived level of mental health (scale of 1 to 5) | ... not applicable | 0.87 Table 2 Note ** | 0.85 Table 2 Note ** |
Life satisfaction | Level of life satisfaction (scale of 0 to 10) | ... not applicable | 0.99 | 0.98 |
Methodology
This article uses data from the third wave of the Survey Series on People and their Communities (SSPC): Quality of Life, Source of Information and Trust and the 2022 iteration of the Canadian Internet Use Survey (CIUS). The SSPC is a panel survey, whereas every iteration of the CIUS has an independent sample. Both surveys have a target population of individuals aged 15 years and older living in the 10 provinces of Canada, excluding residents of Indigenous reserves.
Data for the CIUS were collected from December 2022 to April 2023. The sample size was approximately 55,700 individuals, and the response rate was 45.3%.
Wave 3 of the SSPC was collected in October 2023. The sample size of the SSPC was approximately 70,000 individuals, and the response rate for wave 3 was 14.4%.
All estimates presented in this paper were weighted to be representative of the target population.
In the regressions, the scales used for the following variables were inverted when compared with the questionnaire:
- SSPC: frequency of fact-checking news or information (dependent variable), hopefulness, loneliness and misinformation concern
- CIUS: frequency of sharing online news or information without fact-checking (dependent variable), seen misinformation and mental health
Acknowledgments
The authors would like to thank several of their Statistics Canada colleagues, including Mark Uhrbach, Simon-Pierre Lacasse and analysts from the Centre for Indigenous Statistics and Partnerships for their guidance in writing this paper. The authors would also like to thank their colleagues from the Privy Council Office for their feedback on earlier versions of this paper.
References
Andrey, S. (2023). Survey of Online Harms in Canada. The Dais at Toronto Metropolitan University. Retrieved May 31, 2024.
Arin, K.P., Mazrekaj, D. & Thum, M. (2023). Ability of detecting and willingness to share fake news. Scientific Reports. 13, 7298. Retrieved May 31, 2024.
Canadian Association of Science Centres (2023). Information Overload: Exploring the challenges people face navigating a sea of information. Retrieved May 31, 2024.
Chong, S.K. , Ali, S.H. , Ðoàn, L.N. , Yi, S.S., Trinh-Shevrin, C. & Kwon, S.C. (2022). Social Media Use and Misinformation Among Asian Americans During COVID-19. Frontiers in Public Health. 9 – 2021. Retrieved May 31, 2024.
Employment Equity Act, S.C. 1995, c. 44. Retrieved May 31, 2024.
Government of Canada (2024). Learn about online disinformation. Retrieved May 31, 2024.
Government of Canada (2024). Online disinformation. Retrieved May 31, 2024.
Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development (2024). The OECD Truth Quest Survey: Methodology and findings. OECD Digital Economy Papers. No. 369. OECD Publishing. Retrieved July 2, 2024.
Preston, S., Anderson, A., Robertson, D.J., Shephard, M.P. & Huhe, N. (2021). Detecting fake news on Facebook: The role of emotional intelligence. PLOS ONE. 16(3): e0246757. Retrieved May 31, 2024.
Statistics Canada (2023). Canadian Social Survey - Quality of Life, Virtual Health Care and Trust, 2023. The Daily. Retrieved May 31, 2024.
The Constitution Acts, 1867 to 1982. Part II. Retrieved May 31, 2024.
UCLA: Statistical Consulting Group. FAQ: How do I interpret odds ratios in logistic regression? University of California, Los Angeles. Retrieved May 31, 2024.
UCLA: Statistical Consulting Group. Ordinal Logistic Regression | R Data Analysis Examples. University of California, Los Angeles. Retrieved May 31, 2024.
- Date modified: